Текст книги "Кости, гены и культура"
Автор книги: Александр Марков
Жанр: Прочая образовательная литература, Наука и Образование
Возрастные ограничения: +12
сообщить о неприемлемом содержимом
Текущая страница: 35 (всего у книги 36 страниц)
Список литературы
Марков А. В., Наймарк Е. Б. 2014. Эволюция. Классические идеи в свете новых открытий. М.: Corpus.
Марков А. В., Наймарк Е. Б. 2019. Перспективы отбора. От зеленых пеночек и бессмысленного усложнения до голых землекопов и мутирующего человечества. М.: Corpus.
Медникова М. Б. 2011. К антропологии древнейшего населения Алтая: проксимальная фаланга стопы из раскопок Денисовой пещеры // Археология, этнография и антропология Евразии. № 1. С. 129–138.
Резникова Ж. И. 2004. Сравнительный анализ различных форм социального обучения у животных // Журнал общей биологии. Т. 65. С. 136–152.
Резникова Ж. И. 2009. Социальное обучение у животных // Природа. № 5. C. 3–12.
Сапольски Р. 2019. Биология добра и зла. Как наука объясняет наши поступки. М.: Альпина нон-фикшн.
Соколов А. 2020. Странная обезьяна. Куда делась шерсть и почему люди разного цвета. М.: Альпина нон-фикшн.
Abi-Rached L. et al. 2011. The shaping of modern human immune systems by multiregional admixture with archaic humans // Science. V. 334. P. 89–94.
Alem S. et al. 2016. Associative mechanisms allow for social learning and cultural transmission of string pulling in an insect // PLoS Biology. V. 14. e1002564.
Alexander R. D. 1989. Evolution of the human psyche / Mellars P., Stringer C., eds. The human revolution. P. 454–513.
Allentoft M. E. et al. 2015. Population genomics of Bronze Age Eurasia // Nature. V. 522. P. 167–172.
Andics A. et al. 2016. Neural mechanisms for lexical processing in dogs // Science. V. 353. P. 1030–1032.
Aplin L. M. et al. 2015. Experimentally induced innovations lead to persistent culture via conformity in wild birds // Nature. V. 518. P. 538–541.
Arsuaga J. L. et al. 2014. Neandertal roots: cranial and chronological evidence from Sima de los Huesos // Science. V. 344. P. 1358–1363.
Ashton B. J. et al. 2018. Cognitive performance is linked to group size and affects fitness in Australian magpies // Nature. V. 554. P. 364–367.
Aubert M. et al. 2014. Pleistocene cave art from Sulawesi, Indonesia // Nature. V. 514. P. 223–227.
Aubert M. et al. 2018. Palaeolithic cave art in Borneo // Nature. V. 564. P. 254–257.
Aubert M. et al. 2019. Earliest hunting scene in prehistoric art // Nature. V. 576. P. 442–445.
Bae C. J. et al. 2017. On the origin of modern humans: Asian perspectives // Science. V. 358. eaai9067.
Barton R. A., Harvey P. H. 2000. Mosaic evolution of brain structure in mammals // Nature. V. 405. P. 1055–1058.
Bednarik R., Beaumont P. 2012. Pleistocene engravings from Wonderwerk Cave, South Africa // Préhistoire, Art et Sociétés, Bulletin de la Société Préhistorique Ariège-Pyrénées. V. 65–66. P. 96–97.
Beier J. et al. 2018. Similar cranial trauma prevalence among Neanderthals and Upper Palaeolithic modern humans // Nature. V. 563. P. 686–690.
Benazzi S. et al. 2015. The makers of the Protoaurignacian and implications for Neandertal extinction // Science. V. 348. P. 793–796.
Berger L. R. et al. 2015. Homo naledi, a new species of the genus Homo from the Dinaledi Chamber, South Africa // eLife. V. 4. e09560.
Berger L. R. et al. 2017. Homo naledi and Pleistocene hominin evolution in subequatorial Africa // eLife. V. 6. e24234.
Berger T. D., Trinkaus E. 1995. Patterns of trauma among the Neandertals // Journal of Archaeological Science. V. 22. P. 841–852.
Berna F. et al. 2012. Microstratigraphic evidence of in situ fire in the Acheulean strata of Wonderwerk Cave, Northern Cape province, South Africa // PNAS. V. 109. P. 7593–7594.
Beyene Y. et al. 2013. The characteristics and chronology of the earliest Acheulean at Konso, Ethiopia // PNAS. V. 5. P. 1584–1591.
Bickerton D., Szathmary E. 2011. Confrontational scavenging as a possible source for language and cooperation // BMC Evolutionary Biology. V. 11. P. 261.
Bon F. 2006. A brief overview of Aurignacian cultures in the context of the industries of the transition from the Middle to the Upper Paleolithic / Bar-Yosef O., ZilhÃo J., eds. Towards the definition of the Aurignacian. Lisbon: Instituto Português de Arqueologia. P. 133–144.
Braun D. R. et al. 2010. Early hominin diet included diverse terrestrial and aquatic animals 1.95 Ma in East Turkana, Kenya // PNAS. V. 107. P. 10002–10007.
Brooks A. S. et al. 2018. Long-distance stone transport and pigment use in the earliest Middle Stone Age // Science. V. 360. P. 90–94.
Brown S. et al. 2016. Identification of a new hominin bone from Denisova Cave, Siberia using collagen fingerprinting and mitochondrial DNA analysis // Scientific Reports. V. 6. 23559.
Browning S. R. et al. 2018. Analysis of human sequence data reveals two pulses of archaic Denisovan admixture // Cell. V. 173. P. 1–9.
Brumm A. et al. 2010. Hominins on Flores, Indonesia, by one million years ago // Nature. V. 464. P. 748–752.
Brumm A. et al. 2016. Age and context of the oldest known hominin fossils from Flores // Nature. V. 534. P. 249–253.
Buckley M. 2018. Zooarchaeology by mass spectrometry (ZooMS) collagen fingerprinting for the species identification of archaeological bone fragments / Giovas C., LeFebvre M., eds. Zooarchaeology in practice. Cham: Springer. P. 227–247.
Burger J. et al. 2020. Low prevalence of lactase persistence in Bronze Age Europe indicates ongoing strong selection over the last 3,000 years // Current Biology. V. 30. P. 4307–4315.
Byrne R. W., Whiten A. 1988. Machiavellian intelligence: social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes and humans. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Byrne R. W., Corp N. 2004. Neocortex size predicts deception rate in primates // Proceedings of the Royal Society B. V. 271. P. 1693–1699.
Callaway E. 2019. First portrait of mysterious Denisovans drawn from DNA // Nature. V. 573. P. 475–476.
Caro T. M., Hauser M. D. 1992. Is there teaching in nonhuman animals? // The Quarterly Review of Biology. V. 67. P. 151–174.
Caspari R., Lee S.-H. 2004. Older age becomes common late in human evolution // PNAS. V. 101. P. 10895–10900.
Cerling T. E. et al. 2013. Stable isotope-based diet reconstructions of Turkana Basin hominins // PNAS. V. 110. P. 10501–10506.
Chang C.-H. et al. 2015. The first archaic Homo from Taiwan // Nature Communications. V. 6. P. 6037.
Chen F. et al. 2019. A late Middle Pleistocene Denisovan mandible from the Tibetan Plateau // Nature. V. 569. P. 409–412.
Crews D. E. 2003. Human senescence: evolutionary and biocultural perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Curtis J. T. et al. 2006. Dopamine and monogamy // Brain Research. V. 1126. P. 76–90.
Dabney J. et al. 2013. Complete mitochondrial genome sequence of a Middle Pleistocene cave bear reconstructed from ultrashort DNA fragments // PNAS. V. 110. P. 15758–15763.
Dear K. et al. 2019. Do “watching eyes” influence antisocial behavior? A systematic review & meta-analysis // Evolution and Human Behavior. V. 40. P. 269–280.
DeCasien A. R., Higham J. P. 2019. Primate mosaic brain evolution reflects selection on sensory and cognitive specialization // Nature Ecology & Evolution. V. 3. P. 1483–1493.
De Dreu C. K. et al. 2012. Oxytocin motivates non-cooperation in intergroup conflict to protect vulnerable in-group members // PLoS ONE. V. 7. e46751.
De Dreu C. K. 2016. Oxytocin conditions intergroup relations through upregulated in-group empathy, cooperation, conformity, and defense // Biological Psychiatry. V. 79. P. 165–173.
Deino A. L. et al. 2018. Chronology of the Acheulean to Middle Stone Age transition in eastern Africa // Science. V. 360. P. 95–98.
De Manuel M. et al. 2016. Chimpanzee genomic diversity reveals ancient admixture with bonobos // Science. V. 354. P. 477–481.
DÉtroit F. et al. 2019. A new species of Homo from the Late Pleistocene of the Philippines // Nature. V. 568. P. 181–186.
Devoogd T. et al. 1993. Relations between song repertoire size and the volume of brain nuclei related to song: comparative evolutionary analyses amongst oscine birds // Proceedings of the Royal Society B. V. 254. P. 75–82.
De Waal F. 2001. The ape and the sushi master: cultural reflections of a primatologist. New York: Basic Books.
DiMaggio E. N. et al. 2015. Late Pliocene fossiliferous sedimentary record and the environmental context of early Homo from Afar, Ethiopia // Science. V. 347. P. 1355–1359.
Dirks P. H. G. M. et al. 2015. Geological and taphonomic context for the new hominin species Homo naledi from the Dinaledi Chamber, South Africa // eLife. V. 4. e09561.
Dirks P. H. G. M. et al. 2017. The age of Homo naledi and associated sediments in the Rising Star Cave, South Africa // eLife. V. 6. e24231.
Douka K. et al. 2019. Age estimates for hominin fossils and the onset of the Upper Palaeolithic at Denisova Cave // Nature. V. 565. P. 640–644.
Driscoll C. A. et al. 2007. The Near Eastern origin of cat domestication // Science. V. 317. P. 519–523.
Durvasula A., Sankararaman S. 2020. Recovering signals of ghost archaic introgression in African populations // Science Advances. V. 6. eaax5097.
Enquist M., Ghirlanda S. 2007. Evolution of social learning does not explain the origin of human cumulative culture // Journal of Theoretical Biology. V. 246. P. 129–135.
FalÓtico T., Ottoni E. B. 2013. Stone throwing as a sexual display in wild female bearded capuchin monkeys, Sapajus libidinosus // PLoS ONE. V. 8. e79535.
FalÓtico T. et al. 2019. Three thousand years of wild capuchin stone tool use // Nature Ecology & Evolution. V. 3. P. 1034–1038.
Faurby S. et al. 2020. Brain expansion in early hominins predicts carnivore extinctions in East Africa // Ecology Letters. V. 23. P. 537–544.
Fedorova N. et al. 2017. Living in stable social groups is associated with reduced brain size in woodpeckers (Picidae) // Biology Letters. V. 13. 20170008.
Flegontov P. et al. 2019. Palaeo-Eskimo genetic ancestry and the peopling of Chukotka and North America // Nature. V. 570. P. 236–240.
Fox K. C. R. et al. 2017. The social and cultural roots of whale and dolphin brains // Nature Ecology & Evolution. V. 1. P. 1699–1705.
Franks N. R., Richardson T. 2006. Teaching in tandem-running ants // Nature. V. 439. P. 153.
Fu Q. et al. 2013. DNA analysis of an early modern human from Asia // PNAS. V. 110. P. 2223–2227.
Fu Q. et al. 2014. Genome sequence of a 45,000-year-old modern human from western Siberia // Nature. V. 514. P. 445–449.
Fu Q. et al. 2015. An early modern human from Romania with a recent Neanderthal ancestor // Nature. V. 524. P. 216–219.
Fu Q. et al. 2016. The genetic history of Ice Age Europe// Nature. V. 534. P. 200–205.
Gavrilets S. 2015. Collective action and the collaborative brain // Journal of the Royal Society Interface. V. 12. 20141067.
Gavrilets S., Vose A. 2006. The dynamics of Machiavellian intelligence // PNAS. V. 103. P. 16823–16828.
Gibbons A. 2013. Stunning skull gives a fresh portrait of early humans // Science. V. 342. P. 297–298.
Gokhman D. et al. 2014. Reconstructing the DNA methylation maps of the Neandertal and the Denisovan // Science. V. 344. P. 523–527.
Gokhman D. et al. 2019. Reconstructing Denisovan anatomy using DNA methylation maps // Cell. V. 179. P. 180–192.
GÓmez J. M. et al. 2016. The phylogenetic roots of human lethal violence // Nature. V. 538. P. 233–237.
Goodson J. L. 2005. The vertebrate social behavior network: evolutionary themes and variations // Hormones and Behavior. V. 48. P. 11–22.
Goriounova N. A., Mansvelder H. D. 2019. Genes, cells and brain areas of intelligence // Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. V. 13. P. 44.
GrÜn R., Schwarcz H. P. 1988. ESR dating of tooth enamel: coupled correction for U-uptake and U-series disequilibrium // International Journal of Radiation Applications and Instrumentation. Part D. Nuclear Tracks and Radiation Measurements. V. 14. P. 237–241.
GrÜn R. et al. 2020. Dating the skull from Broken Hill, Zambia, and its position in human evolution // Nature. V. 580. P. 372–375.
Guillette L. M. et al. 2016. Social learning in nest-building birds: a role for familiarity // Proceedings of the Royal Society B. V. 283. 20152685.
Gunz et al. 2019. Neandertal introgression sheds light on modern human endocranial globularity // Current Biology. V. 29. P. 1–8.
Haak W. et al. 2015. Massive migration from the steppe was a source for Indo-European languages in Europe // Nature. V. 522. P. 207–211.
Hajdinjak M. et al. 2018. Reconstructing the genetic history of late Neanderthals // Nature. V. 555. P. 652–656.
Harmand S. et al. 2015. 3.3-million-year-old stone tools from Lomekwi 3, West Turkana, Kenya // Nature. V. 521. P. 310–315.
Hawks J. et al. 2017. New fossil remains of Homo naledi from the Lesedi Chamber, South Africa // eLife. V. 6. e24232.
Henrich J. 2015. The secret of our success: how culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Henshilwood C. S. et al. 2011. A 100,000-year-old ochre-processing workshop at Blombos Cave, South Africa // Science. V. 334. P. 219–222.
Hershkovitz I. et al. 2018. The earliest modern humans outside Africa // Science. V. 359. P. 456–459.
Heyes C. 2012. New thinking: the evolution of human cognition // Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. V. 367. P. 2091–2096.
Hill J. et al. 2010. Similar patterns of cortical expansion during human development and evolution // PNAS. V. 107. P. 13135–13140.
Hobaiter C. et al. 2014. Social network analysis shows direct evidence for social transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees // PLoS Biology. V. 12. e1001960.
Holloway R. L. 2015. The evolution of the hominid brain / Henke W., Tattersall I., eds. Handbook of paleoanthropology. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer. P. 1961–1987.
HorvÁth K. et al. 2011. Is the social brain theory applicable to human individual differences? Relationship between sociability personality dimension and brain size // Evolutionary Psychology. V. 9. P. 244–256.
Hsieh P. et al. 2016. Model-based analyses of whole-genome data reveal a complex evolutionary history involving archaic introgression in Central African Pygmies // Genome Research. V. 26. P. 291–300.
Hublin J.-J. et al. 2015. Brain ontogeny and life history in Pleistocene hominins // Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. V. 370. 20140062.
Hublin J.-J. et al. 2017. New fossils from Jebel Irhoud, Morocco and the pan-African origin of Homo sapiens // Nature. V. 546. P. 289–292.
Humphrey N. K. 1976. The social function of intellect / Bateson P. P. G., Hinde R. A., eds. Growing points in ethology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. P. 303–317.
Ingicco T. et al. 2018. Earliest known hominin activity in the Philippines by 709 thousand years ago // Nature. V. 557. P. 233–237.
Jacobs G. S. et al. 2019. Multiple deeply divergent Denisovan ancestries in Papuans // Cell. V. 177. P. 1–12.
Jacobs Z. et al. 2019. Timing of archaic hominin occupation of Denisova Cave in southern Siberia // Nature. V. 565. P. 594–599.
Jesmer B. R. et al. 2018. Is ungulate migration culturally transmitted? Evidence of social learning from translocated animals // Science. V. 361. P. 1023–1025.
Johnstone B. et al. 2012. Right parietal lobe-related “selflessness” as the neuropsychological basis of spiritual transcendence // The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion. V. 22. P. 267–284.
Joordens J. C. A. et al. 2009. Relevance of aquatic environments for hominins: a case study from Trinil (Java, Indonesia) // Journal of Human Evolution. V. 57. P. 656–671.
Joordens J. C. A. et al. 2014. Homo erectus at Trinil on Java used shells for tool production and engraving // Nature. V. 518. P. 228–231.
Kaplan H. S., Robson A. J. 2002. The emergence of humans: the coevolution of intelligence and longevity with intergenerational transfers // PNAS. V. 99. P. 10221–10226.
Kipling R. 1902. The cat that walked by himself / Kipling R. Just so stories. London: Macmillan and Co. [Киплинг Р. 1929. Кошка, гулявшая сама по себе. М.-Л.: Государственное издательство.]
KorleviĆ P. et al. 2015. Reducing microbial and human contamination in DNA extractions from ancient bones and teeth // Biotechniques. V. 59. P. 87–93.
Krebs J. R. 1990. Food-storing birds: adaptive specialization in brain and behaviour? // Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. V. 329. P. 153–160.
KÜhl H. S. et al. 2016. Chimpanzee accumulative stone throwing // Scientific Reports. V. 6. 22219.
KÜhl H. S. et al. 2019. Human impact erodes chimpanzee behavioral diversity // Science. V. 363. P. 1453–1455.
Kuhlwilm M. et al. 2016. Ancient gene flow from early modern humans into Eastern Neanderthals // Nature. V. 530. P. 429–433.
Lachance J. et al. 2012. Evolutionary history and adaptation from high-coverage whole-genome sequences of diverse African hunter-gatherers // Cell. V. 150. P. 457–469.
Laland K. N. 2017. Darwin’s unfinished symphony. How culture made the human mind. Princeton & Oxford: Princeton University Press.
Laland K. N., O’Brien M. J. 2011. Cultural niche construction: an introduction // Biological Theory. V. 6. P. 191–202.
Laland K. N. et al. 2001. Cultural niche construction and human evolution // Journal of Evolutionary Biology. V. 14. P. 22–33.
Laland K. N., Rendell L. 2013. Cultural memory // Current Biology. V. 23. P. R736–R740.
Leca J.-B. et al. 2010. Indirect social influence in the maintenance of the stone-handling tradition in Japanese macaques, Macaca fuscata // Animal Behaviour. V. 79. P. 117–126.
Leigh R. S. 2012. Brain size growth and life history in human evolution // Evolutionary Biology. V. 39. P. 587–599.
Lewis H. M., Laland K. N. 2012. Transmission fidelity is the key to the build-up of cumulative culture // Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. V. 367. P. 2171–2180.
Li H., Durbin R. 2011. Inference of human population history from individual whole-genome sequences // Nature. V. 475. P. 493–496.
Li Z.-Y. et al. 2017. Late Pleistocene archaic human crania from Xuchang, China // Science. V. 355. P. 969–972.
Lordkipanidze D. et al. 2013. A complete skull from Dmanisi, Georgia, and the evolutionary biology of early Homo // Science. V. 342. P. 326–331.
Loukola O. J. et al. 2016. Bumblebees show cognitive flexibility by improving on an observed complex behavior // Science. V. 355. P. 833–836.
Ludwig A. et al. 2009. Coat color variation at the beginning of horse domestication // Science. V. 324. P. 485.
Maestripieri D. 1995. Maternal encouragement in nonhuman primates and the question of animal teaching // Human Nature. V. 6. P. 361–378.
Maguire E. A. et al. 2000. Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers // PNAS. V. 97. P. 4398–4403.
Maklakov A. A. et al. 2011. Brains and the city: big-brained passerine birds succeed in urban environments // Biology Letters. 2011. V. 7. P. 730–732.
Malaivijitnond S. et al. 2007. Stone-tool usage by Thai long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) // American Journal of Primatology. V. 69. P. 227–233.
Malaspinas A.-S. et al. 2016. A genomic history of Aboriginal Australia // Nature. V. 538. P. 207–214.
Mallick S. et al. 2016. The Simons Genome Diversity Project: 300 genomes from 142 diverse populations // Nature. V. 538. P. 201–206.
Maricic T. et al. 2013. A recent evolutionary change affects a regulatory element in the human FOXP2 gene // Molecular Biology and Evolution. V. 30. P. 844–852.
Markov A. V., Markov M. A. 2020. Runaway brain-culture coevolution as a reason for larger brains: exploring the “cultural drive” hypothesis by computer modeling // Ecology and Evolution. V. 10. P. 6059–6077.
Marui W. et al. 2003. Degeneration of tyrosine hydroxylase-immunoreactive neurons in the cerebral cortex and hippocampus of patients with dementia with Lewy bodies // Neuroscience Letters. V. 340. P. 185–188.
McLean C. Y. et al. 2011. Human-specific loss of regulatory DNA and the evolution of human-specific traits // Nature. V. 471. P. 216–219.
Meyer M. et al. 2012. A high-coverage genome sequence from an archaic Denisovan individual // Science. V. 338. P. 222–226.
Meyer M. et al. 2013. A mitochondrial genome sequence of a hominin from Sima de los Huesos // Nature. V. 505. P. 403–406.
Meyer M. et al. 2016. Nuclear DNA sequences from the Middle Pleistocene Sima de los Huesos hominins // Nature. V. 531. P. 504–507.
Mijares A. S. et al. 2010. New evidence for a 67,000-year-old human presence at Callao Cave, Luzon, Philippines // Journal of Human Evolution. V. 59. P. 123–132.
Millard A. R. 2008. A critique of the chronometric evidence for hominid fossils: I. Africa and the Near East 500–50 ka // Journal of Human Evolution. V. 54. P. 848–874.
Miller G. F. 2000. The mating mind. How sexual choice shaped the evolution of human nature. New York: Doubleday. [Миллер Д. 2020. Соблазняющий разум. М.: Corpus.]
Miller I. F. et al. 2019. Quantitative uniqueness of human brain evolution revealed through phylogenetic comparative analysis // eLife. V. 8. e41250.
Moreno-Mayar J. V. et al. 2018. Terminal Pleistocene Alaskan genome reveals first founding population of Native Americans // Nature. V. 553. P. 203–207.
Morand-Ferron J. et al. 2016. Studying the evolutionary ecology of cognition in the wild: a review of practical and conceptual challenges // Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society. V. 91. P. 367–389.
Morgan T. J. H. et al. 2015. Experimental evidence for the co-evolution of hominin tool-making, teaching and language // Nature Communications. V. 6. 6029.
Moura A. C. de A., Lee P. C. 2004. Capuchin stone tool use in Caatinga dry forest // Science. V. 306. P. 1909.
Mueller T. et al. 2013. Social learning of migratory performance // Science. V. 341. P. 999–1002.
Musgrave S. et al. 2016. Tool transfers are a form of teaching among chimpanzees // Scientific Reports. V. 6. 34783.
Muthukrishna M. et al. 2018. The cultural brain hypothesis: how culture drives brain expansion, sociality, and life history // PLoS Computational Biology. V. 14. e1006504.
Navarrete A. F. et al. 2016. The coevolution of innovation and technical intelligence in primates // Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. V. 371. 20150186.
Neubauer S. et al. 2018. The evolution of modern human brain shape // Science Advances. V. 4. eaao5961.
Nielsen R. et al. 2014. Altitude adaptation in Tibetans caused by introgression of Denisovan-like DNA // Nature. V. 512. P. 194–197.
Novembre J. 2015. Ancient DNA steps into the language debate // Nature. V. 522. P. 164–165.
O’Connell L. A., Hofmann H. A. 2012. Evolution of a vertebrate social decision-making network // Science. V. 366. P. 1154–1157.
O’Donnell S. et al. 2015. Distributed cognition and social brains: reductions in mushroom body investment accompanied the origins of sociality in wasps (Hymenoptera: Vespidae) // Proceedings of the Royal Society B. V. 282. 20150791.
Orlando L. et al. 2013. Recalibrating Equus evolution using the genome sequence of an early Middle Pleistocene horse // Nature. V. 499. P. 74–78.
Ottoni C. et al. 2017. The palaeogenetics of cat dispersal in the ancient world // Nature Ecology & Evolution. V. 1. 0139.
PÄÄbo S. 1985. Molecular cloning of Ancient Egyptian mummy DNA // Nature. V. 314. P. 644–645.
Pagani L. et al. 2016. Genomic analyses inform on migration events during the peopling of Eurasia // Nature. V. 538. P. 238–242.
ParÉs J. M. et al. 2014. Early human settlements in Northern Africa: paleomagnetic evidence from the Ain Hanech Formation (northeastern Algeria) // Quaternary Science Reviews. V. 99. P. 203–209.
Patterson D. B. et al. 2019. Comparative isotopic evidence from East Turkana supports a dietary shift within the genus Homo // Nature Ecology & Evolution. V. 3. P. 1048–1056.
Pika S. et al. 2019. Wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes troglodytes) exploit tortoises (Kinixys erosa) via percussive technology // Scientific Reports. V. 9. 7661.
Pike A. W. G. et al. 2012. U-Series dating of paleolithic art in 11 caves in Spain // Science. V. 336. P. 1409–1413.
Pitulko V. V. et al. 2004. The Yana RHS site: humans in the Arctic before the last glacial maximum // Science. V. 303. P. 52–56.
Pitulko V. V. et al. 2016. Early human presence in the Arctic: evidence from 45,000-year-old mammoth remains // Science. V. 351. P. 260–263.
Plagnol V., Wall J. D. 2006. Possible ancestral structure in human populations // PLoS Genetics. V. 2. e105.
Plummer T. 2004. Flaked stones and old bones: biological and cultural evolution at the dawn of technology // American Journal of Physical Anthropology. V. 125. P. 118–164.
Potter B. A. et al. 2014. New insights into Eastern Beringian mortuary behavior: a terminal Pleistocene double infant burial at Upward Sun River // PNAS. V. 111. P. 17060–17065.
Potts R. et al. 2018. Environmental dynamics during the onset of the Middle Stone Age in eastern Africa // Science. V. 360. P. 86–90.
Proffitt T. et al. 2016. Wild monkeys flake stone tools // Nature. V. 539. P. 85–88.
PrÜfer K. et al. 2014. The complete genome sequence of a Neanderthal from the Altai Mountains // Nature. V. 505. P. 43–49.
Raghanti M. A. et al. 2018. A neurochemical hypothesis for the origin of hominids // PNAS. V. 115. P. E1108–E1116.
Raghavan M. et al. 2014. Upper Palaeolithic Siberian genome reveals dual ancestry of Native Americans // Nature. V. 505. P. 87–91.
Raihani N. J., Ridley A. R. 2008. Experimental evidence for teaching in wild pied babblers // Animal Behaviour. V. 75. P. 3–11.
Rasmussen M. et al. 2011. An Aboriginal Australian genome reveals separate human dispersals into Asia // Science. V. 334. P. 94–98.
Rasmussen M. et al. 2014. The genome of a Late Pleistocene human from a Clovis burial site in western Montana // Nature. V. 506. P. 225–229.
Reardon P. K. et al. 2018. Normative brain size variation and brain shape diversity in humans // Science. V. 360. P. 1222–1227.
Reich D. et al. 2012. Reconstructing Native American population history // Nature. V. 488. P. 370–374.
Rendell L. et al. 2010. Why copy others? Insights from the social learning strategies tournament // Science. V. 328. P. 208–213.
Richter D. et al. 2017. The age of the hominin fossils from Jebel Irhoud, Morocco, and the origins of the Middle Stone Age // Nature. V. 546. P. 293–296.
Roebroeks W. et al. 2011. Use of red ochre by early Neandertals // PNAS. V. 109. P. 1889–1894.
Roffman I. et al. 2012. Stone tool production and utilization by bonobo-chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) // PNAS. V. 109. P. 14500–14503.
Rogers A. R. 2019. Legofit: estimating population history from genetic data // BMC Bioinformatics. V. 20. 526.
Rogers A. R. et al. 2020. Neanderthal-Denisovan ancestors interbred with a distantly related hominin // Science Advances. V. 6. eaay5483.
Rogers M. J. et al. 1994. Changing patterns of land use by Plio-Pleistocene hominids in the Lake Turkana Basin // Journal of Human Evolution. V. 27. P. 139–158.
Rolandsen C. M. et al. 2016. On fitness and partial migration in a large herbivore – migratory moose have higher reproductive performance than residents // Oikos. V. 126. P. 547–555.
Rose L., Marshall F. 1996. Meat eating, hominid sociality, and home bases revisited // Current Anthropology. V. 37. P. 307–338.
Sahnouni M. et al. 2018. 1.9-million– and 2.4-million-year-old artifacts and stone tool – cutmarked bones from Ain Boucherit, Algeria // Science. V. 362. P. 1297–1301.
Sallet J. et al. 2011. Social network size affects neural circuits in macaques // Science. V. 334. P. 697–700.
Samuni L. et al. 2017. Oxytocin reactivity during intergroup conflict in wild chimpanzees // PNAS. V. 114. P. 268–273.
Sandom C. et al. 2014. Global late Quaternary megafauna extinctions linked to humans, not climate change // Proceedings of the Royal Society B. V. 281. 20133254.
Sankararaman S. et al. 2014. The genomic landscape of Neanderthal ancestry in present-day humans // Nature. V. 507. P. 354–357.
Sanz C. et al. 2009. Design complexity in termite-fishing tools of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) // Biology Letters. V. 5. P. 293–296.
Schiller F. 1997. The cerebral ventricles. From soul to sink // Archives of Neurology. V. 54. P. 1158–1162.
Schoenemann P. T. 2006. Evolution of the size and functional areas of the human brain // Annual Review of Anthropology. V. 35. P. 379–406.
Schuenemann V. J. et al. 2017. Ancient Egyptian mummy genomes suggest an increase of Sub-Saharan African ancestry in post-Roman periods // Nature Communications. V. 8. 15694.
Segurel L. et al. 2020. Why and when was lactase persistence selected for? Insights from Central Asian herders and ancient DNA // PLoS Biology. V. 18. e3000742.
Shulman R. G. et al. 2004. Energetic basis of brain activity: implications for neuroimaging // Trends in Neurosciences. V. 27. P. 489–495.
Sikora M. et al. 2014. Population genomic analysis of ancient and modern genomes yields new insights into the genetic ancestry of the Tyrolean Iceman and the genetic structure of Europe // PLoS Genetics. V. 10. e1004353.
Sikora M. et al. 2017. Ancient genomes show social and reproductive behavior of early Upper Paleolithic foragers // Science. V. 358. P. 659–662.
Sikora M. et al. 2019. The population history of northeastern Siberia since the Pleistocene // Nature. V. 570. P. 182–188.
Simonti C. N. et al. 2016. The phenotypic legacy of admixture between modern humans and Neanderthals // Science. V. 351. P. 737–741.
Skinner M. M. et al. 2015. Human-like hand use in Australopithecus africanus // Science. V. 347. P. 395–399.
Skoglund P. et al. 2015. Genetic evidence for two founding populations of the Americas // Nature. V. 525. P. 104–108.
Slon V. et al. 2017. A fourth Denisovan individual // Science Advances. V. 3. e1700186.
Slon V. et al. 2018. The genome of the offspring of a Neanderthal mother and a Denisovan father // Nature. V. 561. P. 113–116.
Smith F. A. et al. 2018. Body size downgrading of mammals over the late Quaternary // Science. V. 360. P. 310–313.
Smith T. M. et al. 2018. Wintertime stress, nursing, and lead exposure in Neanderthal children // Science Advances. V. 4. eaau9483.
Sobolewski М. E. et al. 2012. Territoriality, tolerance and testosterone in wild chimpanzees // Animal Behaviour. V. 84. P. 1469–1474.
Правообладателям!
Это произведение, предположительно, находится в статусе 'public domain'. Если это не так и размещение материала нарушает чьи-либо права, то сообщите нам об этом.